5 Lectures given at the 20th Anniversary
Celebration of the founding of Clark University in Worcester, Mass.,
September 1909 by Prof. Dr. Sigmund Freud, LL.D.
Dedicated in gratitude to G. Stanley Hall, Ph.D.,
LL.D., President of Clark University, Professor of Psychology and
EducationI
Lecture 1 : Lecture 2 :
Lecture 3 : Lecture 4 :
Lecture 5
Meine Damen und Herren! [given
in German, Ladies and Gentlemen:] It is a new and somewhat
embarrassing experience for me to appear as lecturer before students
of the New World. I assume that I owe this honor to the association
of my name with the theme of psychoanalysis, and consequently it is
of psychoanalysis that I shall aim to speak. I shall attempt to give
you, in very brief form, an historical survey of the origin and
further development of this new method of research and cure.
Granted that it is a merit to have created
psychoanalysis, it is not my merit. I was a student, busy with the
passing of my last examinations, when another physician of Vienna,
Dr. Joseph Breuer (1842-), made the first application of this method
to the case of an hysterical girl (1880-82) . We must now examine
the history of this case and its treatment, which can be found in
detail in "Studien über Hysterie," ("Studies about Hysteria") later
published by Dr. Breuer and myself (1895).
But first one word. I have noticed, with considerable
satisfaction, that the majority of my hearers do not belong to the
medical profession. Now do not fear that a medical education is
necessary to follow what I shall have to say. We shall now accompany
the doctors a little way, but soon we shall take leave of them and
follow Dr. Breuer on a way which is quite his own.
Dr. Breuer's patient was a girl of twenty-one, of a
high degree of intelligence. She had developed in the course of her
two years' illness a series of physical and mental disturbances
which well deserved to be taken seriously. She had a severe
paralysis of both right extremities, with loss of feeling, and at
times the same affliction of the members of the left side of the
body; disturbance of eye-movements, and much impairment of vision;
difficulty in maintaining the position of the head, an intense
nervous cough, nausea when she attempted to take nourishment, and at
one time for several weeks a loss of the power to drink, in spite of
tormenting thirst. Her power of speech was also diminished, and this
progressed so far that she could neither speak nor understand her
mother tongue; and, finally, she was subject to states of "absence,"
of confusion, delirium, alteration of her whole personality. These
states will later claim our attention.
When one hears of such a case, one does not need to
be a physician to incline to the opinion that we are concerned here
with a serious injury, probably of the brain, for which there is
little hope of cure and which will probably lead to the early death
of the patient. The doctors will tell us, however, that in one type
of case with just as unfavorable symptoms, another, far more
favorable, opinion is justified. When one finds such a series of
symptoms in the case of a young girl whose vital organs (heart,
kidneys) are shown by objective tests to be normal, but who has
suffered from strong emotional disturbances, and when the symptoms
differ in certain finer characteristics from what one might
logically expect, in a case like this, the doctors are not too much
disturbed.
They consider that there is present no organic lesion
of the brain, but that enigmatical state, known since the time of
the Greek physicians ashysteria, which
can simulate a whole series of symptoms of various diseases. They
consider in such a case that the life of the patient is not in
danger and that a restoration to health will probably come about of
itself. The differentiation of such an hysteria from a severe
organic lesion is not always very easy. But we do not need to know
how a differential diagnosis of this kind is made; you may be sure
that the case of Breuer's patient was such that no skillful
physician could fail to diagnose an hysteria.
We may also add a word here from the history of the
case. The illness first appeared while the patient was caring for
her father, whom she tenderly loved, during the severe illness which
led to his death, a task which she was compelled to abandon because
she herself fell ill.
So far it has seemed best to go with the doctors, but
we shall soon part company with them. You must not think that the
outlook of a patient with regard to medical aid is essentially
bettered when the diagnosis points to hysteria rather than organic
disease of the brain. Against the serious brain diseases medical
skill is in most cases powerless, but also in the case of hysterical
affections the doctor can do nothing. He must leave it to benign
nature, when and how his hopeful prognosis will be realized. (I know
that this view no longer holds today, but in the lecture I take
myself and my hearers back to the time before 1880. If things have
become different since that time it has been largely due to the work
the history of which I am sketching.) Accordingly, with the
recognition of the disease as hysteria, little is changed in the
situation of the patient, but there is a great change in the
attitude of the doctor. We can observe that he acts quite
differently toward hystericals than toward patients suffering from
organic diseases. He will not bring the same interests to the former
as to the latter, since their suffering is much less serious and yet
seems to set up the claim to be valued just as seriously.
But there is another motive in this action. The
physician, who through his studies has learned so much that is
hidden from the laity, can realize in his thought the causes and
alterations of the brain disorders in patients suffering from
apoplexy or dementia, a representation which must be right up to a
certain point, for by it he is enabled to understand the nature of
each symptom. But before the details of hysterical symptoms, all his
knowledge, his anatomical-physiological and pathological education,
desert him. He cannot understand hysteria. He is in the same
position before it as the layman. And that is not agreeable to
anyone who is in the habit of setting such a high valuation upon his
knowledge. Hystericals, accordingly, tend to lose his sympathy; he
considers them persons who overstep the laws of his science, as the
orthodox regard heretics; he ascribes to them all possible evils,
blames them for exaggeration and intentional deceit, "simulation,"
and he punishes them by withdrawing his interest.
Now Dr. Breuer did not deserve this reproach in this
case; he gave his patient sympathy and interest, although at first
he did not understand how to help her. Probably this was easier for
him on account of those superior qualities of the patient's mind and
character, to which he bears witness in his account of the case.
His sympathetic observation soon found the means
which made the first help possible. It had been noticed that the
patient, in her states of "absence," of psychic alteration, usually
mumbled over several words to herself. These seemed to spring from
associations with which her thoughts were busy. The doctor, who was
able to get these words, put her in a sort of hypnosis and repeated
them to her over and over, in order to bring up any associations
that they might have. The patient yielded to his suggestion and
reproduced for him those psychic creations which controlled her
thoughts during her "absences," and which betrayed themselves in
these single spoken words.
These were fancies, deeply sad, often poetically
beautiful, day dreams, we might call them, which commonly took as
their starting point the situation of a girl beside the sick-bed of
her father. Whenever she had related a number of such fancies, she
was, as it were, freed and restored to her normal mental life. This
state of health could last for several hours, and then give place on
the next day to a new "absence," which was removed in the same way
by relating the newly created fancies. It was impossible not to get
the impression that the psychic alteration which was expressed in
the "absence" was a consequence of the excitations originating from
these intensely emotional fancy-images. The patient herself, who at
this time of her illness strangely enough understood and spoke only
English, gave this new kind of treatment the name "talking cure," or
jokingly designated it as "chimney-sweeping."
The doctor soon hit upon the fact that through such
cleansing of the soul more could be accomplished than a temporary
removal of the constantly recurring mental "clouds." Symptoms of the
disease would disappear when in hypnosis the patient could be made
to remember the situation and the associative connections under
which they first appeared, provided free vent was given to the
emotions which they aroused.
"There was in the summer a
time of intense heat, and the patient had suffered very much
from thirst; for, without any apparent reason, she had suddenly
become unable to drink. She would take a glass of water in her
hand, but as soon as it touched her lips she would push it away
as though suffering from hydrophobia. Obviously for these few
seconds she was in her absent state. She ate only fruit, melons
and the like, in order to relieve this tormenting thirst. When
this had been going on about six weeks, she was talking one day
in hypnosis about her English governess, whom she disliked, and
finally told, with every sign of disgust, how she had come into
the room of the governess, and how that lady's little dog, that
she abhorred, had drunk out of a glass. Out of respect for the
conventions the patient had remained silent. Now, after she had
given energetic expression to her restrained anger, she asked
for a drink, drank a large quantity of water without trouble,
and woke from hypnosis with the glass at her lips. The symptom
thereupon vanished permanently."
Permit me to dwell for a moment on this experience.
No one had ever cured an hysterical symptom by such means before, or
had come so near understanding its cause. This would be a pregnant
discovery if the expectation could be confirmed that still other,
perhaps the majority of symptoms, originated in this way and could
be removed by the same method. Breuer spared no pains to convince
himself of this and investigated the pathogenesis of the other more
serious symptoms in a more orderly way. Such was indeed the case;
almost all the symptoms originated in exactly this way, as remnants,
as precipitates, if you like, of affectively-toned experiences,
which for that reason we later called "psychic traumata." The nature
of the symptoms became clear through their relation to the scene
which caused them. They were, to use the technical term,
"determined" by the scene whose memory traces they embodied, and so
could no longer be described as arbitrary or enigmatical functions
of the neurosis.
Only one variation from what might be expected must
be mentioned. It was not always a single experience which occasioned
the symptom, but usually several, perhaps many similar, repeated
traumata cooperated in this effect. It was necessary to repeat the
whole series of pathogenic memories in chronological sequence, and
of course in reverse order, the last first and the first last. It
was quite impossible to reach the first and often most essential
trauma directly, without first clearing away those coming later.
You will of course want to hear me speak of other
examples of the causation of hysterical symptoms beside this of
inability to drink on account of the disgust caused by the dog
drinking from the glass. I must, however, if I hold to my program,
limit myself to very few examples. Breuer relates, for instance,
that his patient's visual disturbances could be traced back to
external causes, in the following way:
"The patient, with tears in
her eyes, was sitting by the sick-bed when her father suddenly
asked her what time it was. She could not see distinctly,
strained her eyes to see, brought the watch near her eyes so
that the dial seemed very large, or else she tried hard to
suppress her tears, so that the sick man might not see them."
All the pathogenic impressions sprang from the time
when she shared in the care of her sick father.
"Once she was watching at
night in the greatest anxiety for the patient, who was in a high
fever, and in suspense, for a surgeon was expected from Vienna,
to operate on the patient. Her mother had gone out for a little
while, and Anna sat by the sick-bed, her right arm hanging over
the back of her chair. She fell into a revery and saw a black
snake emerge, as it were, from the wall and approach the sick
man as though to bite him. (It is very probable that several
snakes had actually been seen in the meadow behind the house,
that she had already been frightened by them, and that these
former experiences furnished the material for the
hallucination.) She tried to drive off the creature, but was as
though paralyzed. Her right arm, which was hanging over the back
of the chair, had "gone to sleep," become anaesthetic and
paretic, and as she was looking at it, the fingers changed into
little snakes with death's-heads. (The nails.) Probably she
attempted to drive away the snake with her paralyzed right hand,
and so the anesthesia and paralysis of this member formed
associations with the snake hallucination. When this had
vanished, she tried in her anguish to speak, but could not. She
could not express herself in any language, until finally she
thought of the words of an English nursery song, and thereafter
she could think and speak only in this language."
When the memory of this scene was revived in hypnosis
the paralysis of the right arm, which had existed since the
beginning of the illness, was cured and the treatment ended.
When, a number of years later, I began to use
Breuer's researches and treatment on my own patients, my experiences
completely coincided with his. In the case of a woman of about
forty, there was a tic, a peculiar smacking noise which manifested
itself whenever she was laboring under any excitement, without any
obvious cause. It had its origin in two experiences which had this
common element, that she attempted to make no noise, but that by a
sort of counter-will this noise broke the stillness. On the first
occasion, she had finally after much trouble put her sick child to
sleep, and she tried to be very quiet so as not to awaken it. On the
second occasion, during a ride with both her children in a
thunderstorm the horses took fright, and she carefully avoided any
noise for fear of frightening them still more. I give this example
instead of many others which are cited in the Studien
über Hysterie.
Ladies and gentlemen, if you will permit me to
generalize, as is indispensable in so brief a presentation, we may
express our results up to this point in the formula: Our
hysterical patients suffer from reminiscences. Their symptoms are
the remnants and the memory symbols of certain (traumatic)
experiences.
A comparison with other memory symbols from other
sources will perhaps enable us better to understand this symbolism.
The memorials and monuments with which we adorn our great cities,
are also such memory symbols. If you walk through London you will
find before one of the greatest railway stations of the city a
richly decorated Gothic pillar - "Charing Cross." One of the old
Plantagenet kings, in the thirteenth century, caused the body of his
beloved queen Eleanor to be borne to Westminster, and had Gothic
crosses erected at each of the stations where the coffin was set
down. Charing Cross is the last of these monuments, which preserve
the memory of this sad journey. In another part of the city, you
will see a high pillar of more modern construction, which is merely
called "the Monument." This is in memory of the great fire which
broke out in the neighborhood in the year 1666, and destroyed a
great part of the city. These monuments are memory symbols like the
hysterical symptoms; so far the comparison seems justified. But what
would you say to a Londoner who today stood sadly before the
monument to the funeral of Queen Eleanor, instead of going about his
business with the haste engendered by modern industrial conditions,
or rejoicing with the young queen of his own heart? Or to another,
who before "the Monument" bemoaned the burning of his loved native
city, which long since has arisen again so much more splendid than
before?
Now hystericals and all neurotics behave like these
two unpractical Londoners, not only in that they remember the
painful experiences of the distant past, but because they are still
strongly affected by them. They cannot escape from the past and
neglect present reality in its favor. This fixation of the mental
life on the pathogenic traumata is an essential, and practically a
most significant characteristic of the neurosis.
I will willingly concede the objection which you are
probably formulating, as you think over the history of Breuer's
patient. All her traumata originated at the time when she was caring
for her sick father, and her symptoms could only be regarded as
memory symbols of his sickness and death. They corresponded to
mourning, and a fixation on thoughts of the dead so short a time
after death is certainly not pathological, but rather corresponds to
normal emotional behavior. I concede this: there is nothing abnormal
in the fixation of feeling on the trauma shown by Breuer's patient.
But in other cases, like that of the tic that I have mentioned, the
occasions for which lay ten and fifteen years back, the
characteristic of this abnormal clinging to the past is very clear,
and Breuer's [hysterical] patient would probably have developed it,
if she had not come under the "cathartic treatment" such a short
time after the traumatic experiences and the beginning of the
disease.
We have so far only explained the relation of the
hysterical symptoms to the life history of the patient; now by
considering two further factors which Breuer observed, we may get a
hint as to the processes of the beginning of the illness and those
of the cure. With regard to the first, it is especially to be noted
that Breuer's patient in almost all pathogenic situations had to
suppress a strong excitement, instead of giving vent to it by
appropriate words and deeds. In the little experience with her
governess' dog, she suppressed, through regard for the conventions,
all manifestations of her very intense disgust. While she was seated
by her father's sick bed, she was careful to betray nothing of her
anxiety and her painful depression to the patient. When, later, she
reproduced the same scene before the physician, the emotion which
she had suppressed on the occurrence of the scene burst out with
especial strength, as though it had been pent up all along. The
symptom which had been caused by that scene reached its greatest
intensity while the doctor was striving to revive the memory of the
scene, and vanished after it had been fully laid bare. On the other
hand, experience shows that if the patient is reproducing the
traumatic scene to the physician, the process has no curative effect
if, by some peculiar chance, there is no development of emotion. It
is apparently these emotional processes upon which the illness of
the patient and the restoration to health are dependent.
We feel justified in regarding "emotion" as a
quantity which may become increased, derived and displaced. So we
are forced to the conclusion that the patient fell ill because the
emotion developed in the pathogenic situation was prevented from
escaping normally, and that the essence of the sickness lies in the
fact that these "imprisoned" emotions undergo a series of abnormal
changes. In part they are preserved as a lasting charge and as a
source of constant disturbance in psychical life; in part they
undergo a change into unusual bodily innervations and inhibitions,
which present themselves as the physical symptoms of the case. We
have coined the name "hysterical conversion" for the latter process.
Part of our mental energy is, under normal
conditions, conducted off by way of physical enervation and gives
what we call "the expression of emotions." Hysterical conversion
exaggerates this part of the course of a mental process which is
emotionally colored; it corresponds to a far more intense emotional
expression, which finds outlet by new paths. If a stream flows in
two channels, an overflow of one will take place as soon as the
current in the other meets with an obstacle.
You see that we are in a fair way to arrive at a
purely psychological theory of hysteria, in which we assign the
first rank to the affective processes. A second observation of
Breuer compels us to ascribe to the altered condition of
consciousness a great part in determining the characteristics of the
disease. His patient showed many sorts of mental states, conditions
of "absence," confusion and alteration of character, besides her
normal state. In her normal state she was entirely ignorant of the
pathogenic scenes and of their connection with her symptoms. She had
forgotten those scenes, or at any rate had dissociated them from
their pathogenic connection. When the patient was hypnotized, it was
possible, after considerable difficulty, to recall those scenes to
her memory, and by this means of recall the symptoms were removed.
It would have been extremely perplexing to know how
to interpret this fact, if hypnotic practice and experiments had not
pointed out the way. Through the study of hypnotic phenomena, the
conception, strange though it was at first, has become familiar,
that in one and the same individual several mental groupings are
possible, which may remain relatively independent of each other,
"know nothing" of each other, and which may cause a splitting of
consciousness along lines which they lay down. Cases of such a sort,
known as "double personality", occasionally appear spontaneously. If
in such a division of personality, consciousness remains constantly
bound up with one of the two states, this is called the conscious
mental state, and the other the unconscious.
In the well-known phenomena of so-called post
hypnotic suggestion, in which a command given in hypnosis is later
executed in the normal state as though by an imperative suggestion,
we have an excellent basis for understanding how the unconscious
state can influence the conscious, although the latter is ignorant
of the existence of the former. In the same way it is quite possible
to explain the facts in hysterical cases. Breuer came to the
conclusion that the hysterical symptoms originated in such peculiar
mental states, which he called "hypnoidal states". Experiences of an
emotional nature, which occur during such hypnoidal states easily
become pathogenic, since such states do not present the conditions
for a normal draining off of the emotion of the exciting processes.
And as a result there arises a peculiar product of this exciting
process, that is, the symptom, and this is projected like a foreign
body into the normal state. The latter has, then, no conception of
the hypnoidal pathogenic situation. Where a symptom arises, we also
find an amnesia, a memory gap, and the filling of this gap includes
the removal of the conditions under which the symptom originated.
I am afraid that this portion of my treatment will
not seem very clear, but you must remember that we are dealing here
with new and difficult views, which perhaps could not be made much
clearer. This all goes to show that our knowledge in this field is
not yet far advanced. Breuer's idea of the hypnoidal states has,
moreover, been shown to be superfluous and a hindrance to further
investigation, and has been dropped from present conceptions of
psychoanalysis. Later I shall at least suggest what other influences
and processes have been disclosed besides that of the hypnoidal
states, to which Breuer limited the causal moment.
You have probably also felt, and rightly, that
Breuer's investigations gave you only a very incomplete theory and
insufficient explanation of the phenomena which we have observed.
But complete theories do not fall from Heaven, and you would have
had still greater reason to be distrustful, had any one offered you
at the beginning of his observations a well-rounded theory, without
any gaps; such a theory could only be the child of his speculations
and not the fruit of an unprejudiced investigation of the facts.
II
Ladies and Gentlemen: At about the same time that
Breuer was using the "talking-cure" with his patient, Monsieur
Charcot began in Paris, with the hystericals of the Salpêtrière,
those researches which were to lead to a new understanding of the
disease. These results were, however, not yet known in Vienna. But
when about ten years later Breuer and I published our preliminary
communication on the psychic mechanism of hysterical phenomena,
which grew out of the cathartic treatment of Breuer's first patient,
we were both of us under the spell of Charcot's investigations. We
made the pathogenic experiences of our patients, which acted as
psychic traumata, equivalent to those physical traumata whose
influence on hysterical paralyses Charcot had determined; and
Breuer's hypothesis of hypnoidal states is itself only an echo of
the fact that Charcot had artificially reproduced those traumata
paralyses in hypnosis.
The great French observer, whose student I was during
the years 1885-86, had no natural bent for creating psychological
theories. His student, P. Janet, was the first to attempt to
penetrate more deeply into the psychic processes of hysteria, and we
followed his example, when we made the mental splitting and the
dissociation of personality the central points of our theory. Janet
propounds a theory of hysteria which draws upon the principal
theories of heredity and degeneration which are current in France.
According to his view hysteria is a form of degenerative alteration
of the nervous system, manifesting itself in a congenital "weakness"
of the function of psychic synthesis. The hysterical patient is from
the start incapable of correlating and unifying the manifold of his
mental processes, and so there arises the tendency to mental
dissociation.
If you will permit me to use a banal but clear
illustration, Janet's hysterical reminds one of a weak woman who has
been shopping, and is now on her way home, laden with packages and
bundles of every description. She cannot manage the whole lot with
her two arms and her ten fingers, and soon she drops one. When she
stoops to pick this up, another breaks loose, and so it goes on.
Now it does not agree very well with this assumed
mental weakness of hystericals, that there can be observed in
hysterical cases, besides the phenomena of lessened functioning,
examples of a partial increase of functional capacity, as a sort of
compensation. At the time when Breuer's patient had forgotten her
mother-tongue and all other languages save English, her control of
English attained such a level that if a German book was put before
her she could give a fluent, perfect translation of its contents at
sight. When later I undertook to continue on my own account the
investigations begun by Breuer, I soon came to another view of the
origin of hysterical dissociation (or splitting of consciousness).
It was inevitable that my views should diverge widely
and radically, for my point of departure was not, like that of
Janet, laboratory researches, but attempts at therapy. Above
everything else, it was practical needs that urged me on. The
cathartic treatment, as Breuer had made use of it, presupposed that
the patient should be put in deep hypnosis, for only in hypnosis was
available the knowledge of his pathogenic associations, which were
unknown to him in his normal state.
Now hypnosis, as a fanciful, and so to speak,
mystical, aid, I soon came to dislike; and when I discovered that,
in spite of all my efforts, I could not hypnotize by any means all
of my patients, I resolved to give up hypnotism and to make the
cathartic method independent of it.
Since I could not alter the psychic state of most of
my patients at my wish, I directed my efforts to working with them
in their normal state. This seems at first sight to be a
particularly senseless and aimless undertaking. The problem was
this: to find out something from the patient that the doctor did not
know and the patient himself did not know.
How could one hope to make such a method succeed? The
memory of a very noteworthy and instructive proceeding came to my
aid, which I had seen in Bernheim's clinic at Nancy. Bernheim showed
us that persons put in a condition of hypnotic somnambulism, and
subjected to all sorts of experiences, had only apparently lost the
memory of those somnambulic experiences, and that their memory of
them could be awakened even in the normal state. If he asked them
about their experiences during somnambulism, they said at first that
they did not remember, but if he persisted, urged, assured them that
they did know, then every time the forgotten memory came back.
Accordingly I did this with my patients. When I had
reached in my procedure with them a point at which they declared
that they knew nothing more, I would assure them that they did know,
that they must just tell it out, and I would venture the assertion
that the memory which would emerge at the moment that I laid my hand
on the patient's forehead would be the right one. In this way I
succeeded, without hypnosis, in learning from the patient all that
was necessary for a construction of the connection between the
forgotten pathogenic scenes and the symptoms which they had left
behind.
This was a troublesome and in its length an
exhausting proceeding, and did not lend itself to a finished
technique. But I did not give it up without drawing definite
conclusions from the data which I had gained. I had substantiated
the fact that the forgotten memories were not lost. They were in the
possession of the patient, ready to emerge and form associations
with his other mental content, but hindered from becoming conscious,
and forced to remain in the unconscious by some sort of a force. The
existence of this force could be assumed with certainty, for in
attempting to drag up the unconscious memories into the
consciousness of the patient, in opposition to this force, one got
the sensation of his own personal effort striving to overcome it.
One could get an idea of this force, which maintained the
pathological situation, from the resistance of the patient.
It is on this idea of resistance that I based my
theory of the psychic processes of hystericals. It had been found
that in order to cure the patient it was necessary that this force
should be overcome. Now with the mechanism of the cure as a starting
point, quite a definite theory could be constructed. These same
forces, which in the present situation as resistances opposed the
emergence of the forgotten ideas into consciousness, must themselves
have caused the forgetting, and repressed from consciousness the
pathogenic experiences. I called this hypothetical process
"repression", and considered that it was proved by the undeniable
existence of resistance.
But now the question arose: what were those forces,
and what were the conditions of this repression, in which we were
now able to recognize the pathogenic mechanism of hysteria? A
comparative study of the pathogenic situations, which the cathartic
treatment has made possible, allows us to answer this question. In
all those experiences, it had happened that a wish had been aroused,
which was in sharp opposition to the other desires of the
individual, and was not capable of being reconciled with the
ethical, aesthetic and personal pretensions of the patient's
personality. There had been a short conflict, and the end of this
inner struggle was the repression of the idea which presented itself
to consciousness as the bearer of this irreconcilable wish. This
was, then, repressed from consciousness and forgotten. The
incompatibility of the idea in question with the "ego" of the
patient was the motive of the repression, the ethical and other
pretensions of the individual were the repressing forces. The
presence of the incompatible wish, or the duration of the conflict,
had given rise to a high degree of mental pain; this pain was
avoided by the repression. This latter process is evidently in such
a case a device for the protection of the personality.
I will not multiply examples, but will give you the
history of a single one of my cases, in which the conditions and the
utility of the repression process stand out clearly enough. Of
course for my purpose I must abridge the history of the case and
omit many valuable theoretical considerations. It is that of a young
girl, who was deeply attached to her father, who had died a short
time before, and in whose care she had shared - a situation
analogous to that of Breuer's patient. When her older sister
married, the girl grew to feel a peculiar sympathy for her new
brother-in-law, which easily passed with her for family tenderness.
This sister soon fell ill and died, while the patient and her mother
were away. The absent ones were hastily recalled, without being told
fully of the painful situation.
As the girl stood by the bedside of her dead sister,
for one short moment there surged up in her mind an idea, which
might be framed in these words: "Now he is free and can marry me."
We may be sure that this idea, which betrayed to her consciousness
her intense love for her brother-in-law, of which she had not been
conscious, was the next moment consigned to repression by her
revolted feelings. The girl fell ill with severe hysterical
symptoms, and, when I came to treat the case, it appeared that she
had entirely forgotten that scene at her sister's bedside and the
unnatural, egoistic desire which had arisen in her. She remembered
it during the treatment, reproduced the pathogenic moment with every
sign of intense emotional excitement, and was cured by this
treatment.
Perhaps I can make the process of repression and its
necessary relation to the resistance of the patient, more concrete
by a rough illustration, which I will derive from our present
situation.
Suppose that here in this hall and in this audience,
whose exemplary stillness and attention I cannot sufficiently
commend, there is an individual who is creating a disturbance, and,
by his ill-bred laughing, talking, by scraping his feet, distracts
my attention from my task. I explain that I cannot go on with my
lecture under these conditions, and thereupon several strong men
among you get up, and, after a short struggle, eject the disturber
of the peace from the hall. He is now "repressed," and I can
continue my lecture. But in order that the disturbance may not be
repeated, in case the man who has just been thrown out attempts to
force his way back into the room, the gentlemen who have executed my
suggestion take their chairs to the door and establish themselves
there as a "resistance," to keep up the repression. Now, if you
transfer both locations to the psyche, calling this "consciousness,"
and the outside the "unconscious," you have a tolerably good
illustration of the process of repression.
We can see now the difference between our theory and
that of Janet. We do not derive the psychic fission from a
congenital lack of capacity on the part of the mental apparatus to
synthesize its experiences, but we explain it dynamically by the
conflict of opposing mental forces, we recognize in it the result of
an active striving of each mental complex against the other.
New questions at once arise in great number from our
theory. The situation of psychic conflict is a very frequent one; an
attempt of the ego to defend itself from painful memories can be
observed everywhere, and yet the result is not a mental fission. We
cannot avoid the assumption that still other conditions are
necessary, if the conflict is to result in dissociation. I willingly
concede that with the assumption of "repression" we stand, not at
the end, but at the very beginning of a psychological theory. But we
can advance only one step at a time, and the completion of our
knowledge must await further and more thorough work.
Now do not attempt to bring the case of Breuer's
patient under the point of view of repression. This history cannot
be subjected to such an attempt, for it was gained with the help of
hypnotic influence. Only when hypnosis is excluded can you see the
resistances and repressions and get a correct idea of the pathogenic
process. Hypnosis conceals the resistances and so makes a certain
part of the mental field freely accessible. By this same process the
resistances on the borders of this field are heaped up into a
rampart, which makes all beyond inaccessible.
The most valuable things that we have learned from
Breuer's observations were his conclusions as to the connection of
the symptoms with the pathogenic experiences or psychic traumata,
and we must not neglect to evaluate this result properly from the
standpoint of the repression-theory. It is not at first evident how
we can get from the repression to the creation of the symptoms.
Instead of giving a complicated theoretical derivation, I will
return at this point to the illustration which I used to typify
repression.
Remember that with the ejection of the rowdy and the
establishment of the watchers before the door, the affair is not
necessarily ended. It may very well happen that the ejected man, now
embittered and quite careless of consequences, gives us more to do.
He is no longer among us, we are free from his presence, his
scornful laugh, his half-audible remarks, but in a certain sense the
repression has miscarried, for he makes a terrible uproar outside,
and by his outcries and by hammering on the door with his fists
interferes with my lecture more than before. Under these
circumstances it would be hailed with delight if possibly our
honored president, Dr. Stanley Hall, should take upon himself the
role of peacemaker and mediator. He would speak with the rowdy on
the outside, and then turn to us with the recommendation that we let
him in again, provided he would guarantee to behave himself better.
On Dr. Hall's authority we decide to stop the repression, and now
quiet and peace reign again.
This is in fact a fairly good presentation of the
task devolving upon the physician in the psychoanalytic therapy of
neuroses. To say the same thing more directly: we come to the
conclusion, from working with hysterical patients and other
neurotics, that they have not fully succeeded in repressing the idea
to which the incompatible wish is attached. They have, indeed,
driven it out of consciousness and out of memory, and apparently
saved themselves a great amount of psychic pain, but in the
unconscious the suppressed wish still exists, only waiting for its
chance to become active, and finally succeeds in sending into
consciousness, instead of the repressed idea, a disguised and
unrecognizable surrogate-creation, to which the same painful
sensations associate themselves that the patient thought he was rid
of through his repression.
This surrogate of the suppressed idea - the symptom -
is secure against further attacks from the defenses of the ego, and
instead of a short conflict there originates now a permanent
suffering. We can observe in the symptom, besides the tokens of its
disguise, a remnant of traceable similarity with the originally
repressed idea; the way in which the surrogate is built up can be
discovered during the psychoanalytic treatment of the patient, and
for his cure the symptom must be traced back over the same route to
the repressed idea. If this repressed material is once more made
part of the conscious mental functions, a process which supposes the
overcoming of considerable resistance - the psychic conflict which
then arises, the same which the patient wished to avoid, is made
capable of a happier termination, under the guidance of the
physician, than is offered by repression.
There are several possible suitable decisions which
can bring conflict and neurosis to a happy end; in particular cases
the attempt may be made to combine several of these. Either the
personality of the patient may be convinced that he has been wrong
in rejecting the pathogenic wish, and he may be made to accept it
either wholly or in part; or this wish may itself be directed to a
higher goal which is free from objection, by what is called sublimation; or
the rejection may be recognized as rightly motivated, and the
automatic and therefore insufficient mechanism of repression be
reinforced by the higher, more characteristically human mental
faculties: one succeeds in mastering his wishes by conscious
thought.
Forgive me if I have not been able to present more
clearly these main points of the treatment which is today known as
"psychoanalysis." The difficulties do not lie merely in the newness
of the subject.
Regarding the nature of the unacceptable wishes,
which succeed in making their influence felt out of the unconscious,
in spite of repression; and regarding the question of what
subjective and constitutional factors must be present for such a
failure of repression and such a surrogate or symptom creation to
take place, we will speak in later remarks.
III
Ladies and Gentlemen: It is not always easy to tell
the truth, especially when one must be brief, and so today I must
correct an incorrect statement that I made in my last lecture.
I told you how when I gave up using hypnosis I
pressed my patients to tell me what came into their minds that had
to do with the problem we were working on. I told them that they
would remember what they had apparently forgotten, and that the
thought which irrupted into consciousness would surely embody the
memory for which we were seeking. I claimed that I substantiated the
fact that the first idea of my patients brought the right clew and
could be shown to be the forgotten continuation of the memory. Now
this is not always so; I represented it as being so simple only for
purposes of abbreviation.
In fact, it would only happen the first time that the
right forgotten material would emerge through simple pressure on my
part. If the experience was continued, ideas emerged in every case
which could not be the right ones, for they were not to the purpose,
and the patients themselves rejected them as incorrect. Pressure was
of no further service here, and one could only regret again having
given up hypnosis. In this state of perplexity I clung to a
prejudice which years later was proved by my friend C. G. Jung of
the University of Zurich and his pupils to have a scientific
justification.
I must confess that it is often of great advantage to
have prejudices. I put a high value on the strength of the
determination of mental processes, and I could not believe that any
idea which occurred to the patient, which originated in a state of
concentrated attention, could be quite arbitrary and out of all
relation to the forgotten idea that we were seeking. That it was not
identical with the latter, could be satisfactorily explained by the
hypothetical psychological situation. In the patients whom I treated
there were two opposing forces: on the one hand the conscious
striving to drag up into consciousness the forgotten experience
which was present in the unconscious; and on the other hand the
resistance which we have seen, which set itself against the
emergence of the suppressed idea or its associates into
consciousness.
In case this resistance was nonexistent or very
slight, the forgotten material could become conscious without
disguise. It was then a natural supposition that the disguise would
be more complete, the greater the resistance to the emergence of the
idea. Thoughts which broke into the patient's consciousness instead
of the ideas sought for, were accordingly made up just like
symptoms; they were new, artificial, ephemeral surrogates for the
repressed ideas, and differed from these just in proportion as they
had been more completely disguised under the influence of the
resistances. These surrogates must, however, show a certain
similarity with the ideas which are the object of our search, by
virtue of their nature as symptoms; and when the resistance is not
too intensive it is possible from the nature of these irruptions to
discover the hidden object of our search. This must be related to
the repressed thought as a sort of allusion, as a statement of the
same thing in indirect terms.
We know cases in normal psychology in which analogous
situations to the one which we have assumed give rise to similar
experiences. Such a case is that of wit. By my study of
psychoanalytic technique I was necessarily led to a consideration of
the problem of the nature of wit. I will give one example of this
sort, which, too, is a story that originally appeared in English.
The anecdote runs: Two unscrupulous business men had
succeeded by fortunate speculations in accumulating a large fortune,
and then directed their efforts to breaking into good society. Among
other means they thought it would be of advantage to be painted by
the most famous and expensive artist of the city, a man whose
paintings were considered as events. The costly paintings were first
shown at a great soirée and both hosts led the most influential
connoisseur and art critic to the wall of the salon on which the
portraits were hung, to elicit his admiring judgment. The critic
looked for a long time, looked about as though in search of
something, and then merely asked, pointing out the vacant space
between the two pictures: "And where is the Saviour?"
I see that you are all laughing over this good
example of wit, which we will now attempt to analyze. We understand
that the critic means to say: "You are a couple of malefactors, like
those between whom the Saviour was crucified." But he does not say
this, he expresses himself instead in a way that at first seems not
to the purpose and not related to the matter in hand, but which at
the next moment we recognize as an allusion to the insult at which
he aims, and as a perfect surrogate for it.
We cannot expect to find in the case of wit all those
relations that our theory supposes for the origin of the irruptive
ideas of our patients, but it is my desire to lay stress on the
similar motivation of wit and irruptive idea. Why does not the
critic say directly what he has to say to the two rogues? Because,
in addition to his desire to say it straight out, he is actuated by
strong opposite motives. It is a proceeding which is liable to be
dangerous to offend people who are one's hosts, and who can call to
their aid the strong arms of numerous servants. One might easily
suffer the same fate that I used in the previous lecture to
illustrate repression. On this ground, the critic does not express
the particular insult directly, but in a disguised form, as an
allusion with omission. The same constellation comes into play,
according to our hypothesis, when our patient produces the irruptive
idea as a surrogate for the forgotten idea which is the object of
the quest.
Ladies and Gentlemen, it is very useful to designate
a group of ideas which belong together and have a common emotive
tone, according to the custom of the Zurich school (Bleuler, Jung
and others), as a "complex." So we can say that if we set out from
the last memories of the patient to look for a repressed complex, we
have every prospect of discovering it, if only the patient will
communicate to us a sufficient number of the ideas which come into
his head. So we let the patient speak along any line that he
desires, and cling to the hypothesis that nothing can occur to him
except what has some indirect bearing on the complex that we are
seeking. If this method of discovering the repressed complexes seems
too circumstantial, I can at least assure you that it is the only
available one.
In practicing this technique, one is further bothered
by the fact that the patient often stops, is at a standstill, and
considers that he has nothing to say; nothing occurs to him. If
this were really the case and the patient were right, our procedure
would again be proven inapplicable. Closer observation shows that
such an absence of ideas never really occurs, and that it only
appears to when the patient holds back or rejects the idea which he
perceives, under the influence of the resistance, which disguises
itself as critical judgment of the value of the idea. The patient
can be protected from this if he is warned in advance of this
circumstance, and told to take no account of the critical attitude.
He must say anything that comes into his mind, fully laying aside
such critical choice, even though he may think it unessential,
irrelevant, nonsensical, especially when the idea is one which is
unpleasant to dwell on. By following this prescription we secure the
material which sets us on the track of the repressed complex.
These irruptive ideas, which the patient himself
values little, if he is under the influence of the resistance and
not that of the physician, are for the psychologist like the ore,
which by simple methods of interpretation he reduces from its crude
state to valuable metal. If one desires to gain in a short time a
preliminary knowledge of the patient's repressed complexes, without
going into the question of their arrangement and association, this
examination may be conducted with the help of the association
experiments, as Jung
and his pupils have perfected them. This procedure is to the
psychologist what qualitative analysis is to the chemist; it may be
dispensed with in the therapy of neurotic patients, but is
indispensable in the investigations of the psychoses, which have
been begun by the Zurich school with such valuable results.
This method of work with whatever comes into the
patient's head when he submits to psychoanalytic treatment, is not
the only technical means at our disposal for the widening of
consciousness. Two other methods of procedure serve the same
purpose, the interpretation of his dreams and the evaluation of acts
which he bungles or does without intending to.
I might say, esteemed hearers, that for a long time I
hesitated whether instead of this hurried survey of the whole field
of psychoanalysis, I should not rather offer you a thorough
consideration of the analysis of dreams; a purely subjective and
apparently secondary motive decided me against this. It seemed
rather an impropriety that in this country, so devoted to practical
pursuits, I should pose as "interpreter of dreams," before you had a
chance to discover what significance the old and despised art can
claim.
Interpretation of dreams is in fact the via
regia (royal road)
to the interpretation of the unconscious, the surest ground of
psychoanalysis and a field in which every worker must win his
convictions and gain his education. If I were asked how one could
become a psychoanalyst, I should answer, through the study of his
own dreams. With great tact all opponents of the psychoanalytic
theory have so far either evaded any criticism of the
dream-interpretation or have attempted to pass over it with the most
superficial objections. If, on the contrary, you will undertake the
solution of the problems of dream life, the novelties which
psychoanalysis present to your thoughts will no longer be
difficulties.
You must remember that our nightly dream productions
show the greatest outer similarity and inner relationship to the
creations of the insane, but on the other hand are compatible with
full health during waking life. It does not sound at all absurd to
say that whoever regards these normal sense illusions, these
delusions and alterations of character as matter for amazement
instead of understanding, has not the least prospect of
understanding the abnormal creations of diseased mental states in
any other than the lay sense. You may with confidence place in this
lay group all the psychiatrists of today. Follow me now on a brief
excursion through the field of dream problems.
In our waking state we usually treat dreams with as
little consideration as the patient treats the irruptive ideas which
the psychoanalyst demands from him. It is evident that we reject
them, for we forget them quickly and completely. The slight
valuation which we place on them is based, with those dreams that
are not confused and nonsensical, on the feeling that they are
foreign to our personality, and, with other dreams on their evident
absurdity and senselessness. Our rejection derives support from the
unrestrained shamelessness and the immoral longings which are
obvious in many dreams.
Antiquity, as we know, did not share this light
valuation of dreams. The lower classes of our people today stick
close to the value which they set on dreams; they, however, expect
from them, as did the ancients, the revelation of the future. I
confess that I see no need to adopt mystical hypotheses to fill out
the gaps in our present knowledge, and so I have never been able to
find anything that supported the hypothesis of the prophetic nature
of dreams. Many other things, which are wonderful enough, can be
said about them.
And first, not all dreams are so foreign to the
character of the dreamer, are incomprehensible and confused. If you
will undertake to consider the dreams of young children from the age
of a year and a half on, you will find them quite simple and easy to
interpret. The young child always dreams of the fulfillment of
wishes which were aroused in him the day before and were not
satisfied. You need no art of interpretation to discover this simple
solution, you only need to inquire into the experiences of the child
on the day before (the "dream day"). Now it would certainly be a
most satisfactory solution of the dream-riddle, if the dreams of
adults, too, were the same as those of children, fulfillments of
wishes which had been aroused in them during the dream day. This is
actually the fact; the difficulties which stand in the way of this
solution can be removed step-by-step by a thorough analysis of the
dream.
There is, first of all, the most weighty objection,
that the dreams of adults generally have an incomprehensible
content, which shows wish-fulfillment least of anything. The answer
is this: these dreams have undergone a process of disguise, the
psychic content which underlies them was originally meant for quite
different verbal expression. You must differentiate between the
manifest dream-content, which we remember in the morning only
confusedly, and with difficulty clothe in words which seem
arbitrary, and the latent dream-thoughts, whose presence in the
unconscious we must assume. This distortion of the dream is the same
process which has been revealed to you in the investigations of the
creations (symptoms) of hysterical subjects; it points to the fact
that the same opposition of psychic forces has its share in the
creation of dreams as in the creation of symptoms.
The manifest dream-content is the disguised surrogate
for the unconscious dream thoughts, and this disguising is the work
of the defensive forces of the ego, of the resistances. These
prevent the repressed wishes from entering consciousness during the
waking life, and even in the relaxation of sleep they are still
strong enough to force them to hide themselves by a sort of
masquerading.
The dreamer, then, knows just as little the sense of
his dream as the hysterical knows the relation and significance of
his symptoms. That there are latent dream-thoughts and that between
them and the manifest dream-content there exists the relation just
described - of this you may convince yourselves by the analysis of
dreams, a procedure the technique of which is exactly that of
psychoanalysis.
You must abstract entirely from the apparent
connection of the elements in the manifest dream and seek for the
irruptive ideas which arise through free association, according to
the psychoanalytic laws, from each separate dream element. From this
material the latent dream thoughts may be discovered, exactly as one
divines the concealed complexes of the patient from the fancies
connected with his symptoms and memories. From the latent dream
thoughts which you will find in this way, you will see at once how
thoroughly justified one is in interpreting the dreams of adults by
the same rubrics as those of children. What is now substituted for
the manifest dream-content is the real sense of the dream, is always
clearly comprehensible, associated with the impressions of the day
before, and appears as the fulfilling of an unsatisfied wish. The
manifest dream, which we remember after waking, may then be
described as a disguised fulfillment of repressed wishes.
It is also possible by a sort of synthesis to get
some insight into the process which has brought about the disguise
of the unconscious dream thoughts as the manifest dream-content. We
call this process "dream-work". This deserves our fullest
theoretical interest, since here as nowhere else can we study the
unsuspected psychic processes which are existent in the unconscious,
or, to express it more exactly, between two such separate systems as
the conscious and the unconscious. Among these newly discovered
psychic processes, two,condensation and displacement or
trans-valuation, change of psychic accent, stand out most
prominently.
Dream work is a special case of the reaction of
different mental groupings on each other, and as such is the
consequence of psychic fission. In all essential points it seems
identical with the work of disguise, which changes the repressed
complex in the case of failing repression into symptoms.
You will furthermore discover by the analysis of
dreams, most convincingly your own, the unsuspected importance of
the role which impressions and experiences from early childhood
exert on the development of men.
In the dream life, the child, as it were, continues
his existence in the man, with a retention of all his traits and
wishes, including those which he was obliged to allow to fall into
disuse in his later years. With irresistible might it will be
impressed on you by what processes of development, of repression,
sublimation and reaction there arises out of the child, with its
peculiar gifts and tendencies, the so-called normal man, the bearer
and partly the victim of our painfully acquired civilization.
I will also direct your attention to the fact that we
have discovered from the analysis of dreams that the unconscious
makes use of a sort of symbolism, especially in the presentation of
sexual complexes. This symbolism in part varies with the individual,
but in part is of a typical nature, and seems to be identical with
the symbolism which we suppose to lie behind our myths and legends.
It is not impossible that these later creations of the people may
find their explanation from the study of dreams.
Finally, I must remind you that you must not be led
astray by the objection that the occurrence of anxiety-dreams,
contradicts our idea of the dream as a wish-fulfillment. Apart from
the consideration that anxiety-dreams also require interpretation
before judgment can be passed on them, one can say quite generally
that the anxiety does not depend in such a simple way on the dream
content as one might suppose without more knowledge of the facts,
and more attention to the conditions of neurotic anxiety. Anxiety is
one of the ways in which the ego relieves itself of repressed wishes
which have become too strong and so is easy to explain in the dream,
if the dream has gone too far towards the fulfilling of the
objectionable wish.
You see that the investigation of dreams was
justified by the conclusions which it has given us concerning things
otherwise hard to understand. But we came to it in connection with
the psychoanalytic treatment of neurotics. From what has been said
you can easily understand how the interpretation of dreams, if it is
not made too difficult by the resistance of the patient, can lead to
a knowledge of the patient's concealed and repressed wishes and the
complexes which he is nourishing.
I may now pass to that group of everyday mental
phenomena whose study has become a technical help for
psychoanalysis.
These are the bungling of acts among normal men as
well as among neurotics, to which no significance is ordinarily
attached; the forgetting of things which one is supposed to know and
at other times really does know (for example the temporary
forgetting of proper names); mistakes in speaking, which occur so
frequently; analogous mistakes in writing and in reading, the
automatic execution of purposive acts in wrong situations and the
loss or breaking of objects, etc. These are trifles, for which no
one has ever sought a psychological determination, which have passed
unchallenged as chance experiences, as consequences of
absent-mindedness, inattention and similar conditions. Here, too,
are included the acts and gestures executed without being noticed by
the subject, to say nothing of the fact that he attaches no psychic
importance to them; as playing and trifling with objects, humming
melodies, handling one's person and clothing and the like.
These little things, the bungling of acts, like the
symptomatic and chance acts are not so entirely without meaning as
is generally supposed by a sort of tacit agreement. They have a
meaning, generally easy and sure to interpret from the situation in
which they occur, and it can be demonstrated that they either
express impulses and purposes which are repressed, hidden if
possible from the consciousness of the individual, or that they
spring from exactly the same sort of repressed wishes and complexes
which we have learned to know already as the creators of symptoms
and dreams.
It follows that they deserve the rank of symptoms,
and their observation, like that of dreams, can lead to the
discovery of the hidden complexes of the psychic life. With their
help one will usually betray the most intimate of his secrets. If
these occur so easily and commonly among people in health, with whom
repression has on the whole succeeded fairly well, this is due to
their insignificance and their inconspicuous nature. But they can
lay claim to high theoretic value, for they prove the existence of
repression and surrogate creations even under the conditions of
health.
You have already noticed that the psychoanalyst is
distinguished by an especially strong belief in the determination of
the psychic life. For him there is in the expressions of the psyche
nothing trifling, nothing arbitrary and lawless, he expects
everywhere a widespread motivation, where customarily such claims
are not made; more than that, he is even prepared to find a manifold
motivation of these psychic expressions, while our supposedly inborn
causal need is satisfied with a single psychic cause.
Now keeping in mind the means which we possess for
the discovery of the hidden, forgotten, repressed things in the soul
life: the study of the irruptive ideas called up by free
association, the patient's dreams, and his bungled and symptomatic
acts; and adding to these the evaluation of other phenomena which
emerge during the psychoanalytic treatment, on which I shall later
make a few remarks under the heading of "transfer", you will come
with me to the conclusion that our technique is already sufficiently
efficacious for the solution of the problem of how to introduce the
pathogenic psychic material into consciousness, and so to do away
with the suffering brought on by the creation of surrogate symptoms.
The fact that by such therapeutic endeavors our
knowledge of the mental life of the normal and the abnormal is
widened and deepened, can of course only be regarded as an especial
attraction and superiority of this method.
I do not know whether you have gained the impression
that the technique through whose arsenal I have led you is a
peculiarly difficult one. I consider that on the contrary, for one
who has mastered it, it is quite adapted for use. But so much is
sure, that it is not obvious, that it must be learned no less than
the histological or the surgical technique.
You may be surprised to learn that in Europe we have
heard very frequently judgments passed on psychoanalysis by persons
who knew nothing of its technique and had never practiced it, but
who demanded scornfully that we show the correctness of our results.
There are among these people some who are not in other things
unacquainted with scientific methods of thought, who for example
would not reject the result of a microscopical research because it
cannot be confirmed with the naked eye in anatomical preparations,
and who would not pass judgment until they had used the microscope.
But in matters of psychoanalysis circumstances are really more
unfavorable for gaining recognition. Psychoanalysis will bring the
repressed in mental life to conscious acknowledgment, and every one
who judges it is himself a man who has such repressions, perhaps
maintained only with difficulty. It will consequently call forth the
same resistances from him as from the patient, and this resistance
can easily succeed in disguising itself as intellectual rejection,
and bring forward arguments similar to those from which we protect
our patients by the basic principles of psychoanalysis.
It is not difficult to substantiate in our opponents
the same impairment of intelligence produced by emotivity which we
may observe every day with our patients. The arrogance of
consciousness which for example rejects dreams so lightly, belongs -
quite generally - to the strongest protective apparatus which guards
us against the breaking through of the unconscious complexes, and as
a result it is hard to convince people of the reality of the
unconscious, and to teach them anew what their conscious knowledge
contradicts.
IV
Ladies and Gentlemen: At this point you will be
asking what the technique which I have described has taught us of
the nature of the pathogenic complexes and repressed wishes of
neurotics.
One thing in particular: psychoanalytic
investigations trace back the symptoms of disease with really
surprising regularity to impressions from the sexual life, show us
that the pathogenic wishes are of the nature of erotic
impulse-components, and necessitate the assumption that to
disturbances of the erotic sphere must be ascribed the greatest
significance among the etiological [causative] factors of the
disease. This holds of both sexes.
I know that this assertion will not willingly be
credited. Even those investigators who gladly follow my
psychological labors, are inclined to think that I overestimate the
etiological share of the sexual moments. They ask me why other
mental excitations should not lead to the phenomena of repression
and surrogate-creation which I have described. I can give them this
answer; that I do not know why they should not do this, I have no
objection to their doing it, but experience shows that they do not
possess such a significance, and that they merely support the effect
of the sexual moments, without being able to supplant them.
This conclusion was not a theoretical postulate; in
the Studien Über
Hysterie, published
in 1895 with Dr. Breuer, I did not stand on this ground. I was
converted to it when my experience was richer and had led me deeper
into the nature of the case.
Gentlemen, there are among you some of my closest
friends and adherents, who have traveled to Worcester with me. Ask
them, and they will tell you that they all were at first completely
skeptical of the assertion of the determinative significance of the
sexual etiology, until they were compelled by their own analytic
labors to come to the same conclusion.
The conduct of the patients does not make it any
easier to convince one's self of the correctness of the view which I
have expressed. Instead of willingly giving us information
concerning their sexual life, they try to conceal it by every means
in their power. Men generally are not candid in sexual matters. They
do not show their sexuality freely, but they wear a thick overcoat -
a fabric of lies - to conceal it, as though it were bad weather in
the world of sex. And they are not wrong; sun and wind are not
favorable in our civilized society to any demonstration of sex life.
In truth no one can freely disclose his erotic life to his neighbor.
But when your patients see that in your treatment they may disregard
the conventional restraints, they lay aside this veil of lies, and
then only are you in a position to formulate a judgment of the
question in dispute. Unfortunately physicians are not favored above
the rest of the children of men in their personal relationship to
the questions of the sex life. Many of them are under the ban of
that mixture of prudery and lasciviousness which determines the
behavior of most refined people in affairs of sex.
Now to proceed with the communication of our results.
It is true that in another series of cases psychoanalysis at first
traces the symptoms back not to the sexual, but to banal traumatic
experiences. But the distinction loses its significance through
other circumstances. The work of analysis which is necessary for the
thorough explanation and complete cure of a case of sickness does
not stop in any case with the experience of the time of onset of the
disease, but in every case it goes back to the adolescence and the
early childhood of the patient. Here only do we hit upon the
impressions and circumstances which determine the later sickness.
Only the childhood experiences can give the explanation for the
sensitivity to later traumata and only when these memory traces,
which almost always are forgotten, are discovered and made
conscious, is the power developed to banish the symptoms.
We arrive here at the same conclusion as in the
investigation of dreams - that it is the incompatible, repressed
wishes of childhood which lend their power to the creation of
symptoms. Without these the reactions upon later traumata discharge
normally. But we must consider these mighty wishes of childhood very
generally as sexual in nature.
Now I can, at any rate, be sure of your astonishment.
Is there an infantile sexuality? you will ask. Is childhood not
rather that period of life which is distinguished by the lack of the
sexual impulse? No, Gentlemen, it is not at all true that the sexual
impulse enters into the child at puberty, as the devils in the
gospel entered into the swine. The child has his sexual impulses and
activities from the beginning; he brings them with him into the
world, and from these the so-called normal sexuality of adults
emerges by a significant development through manifold stages. It is
not very difficult to observe the expressions of this childish
sexual activity; it needs rather a certain art to overlook them or
to fail to interpret them.
As fate would have it, I am in a position to call a
witness for my assertions from your own midst. I show you here the
work of one, Dr. Sanford Bell, published in 1902 in the American
Journal of Psychology. The author was a fellow of Clark University,
the same institution within whose walls we now stand. In this
thesis, entitled A
Preliminary Study of the Emotion of Love between the Sexes, which
appeared three years before my Three
Essays on Sexual Theory, the
author says just what I have been saying to you:
"The emotion of sex love . . .
does not make its appearance for the first time at the period of
adolescence as has been thought."
He has, as we should say in Europe, worked by the
American method, and has gathered not less than 2,500 positive
observations in the course of fifteen years, among them 800 of his
own. He says of the signs by which this amorous condition manifests
itself:
"The unprejudiced mind, in
observing these manifestations in hundreds of couples of
children, cannot escape referring them to sex origin. The most
exacting mind is satisfied when to these observations are added
the confessions of those who have as children experienced the
emotion to a marked degree of intensity, and whose memories of
childhood are relatively distinct."
Those of you who are unwilling to believe in
infantile sexuality will be most astonished to hear that among those
children who fell in love so early not a few are of the tender ages
of three, four, and five years.
It would not be surprising if you should believe the
observations of a fellow-countryman rather than my own. Fortunately
a short time ago from the analysis of a five-year-old boy who was
suffering from anxiety, an analysis undertaken with correct
technique by his own father, I succeeded in getting a fairly
complete picture of the bodily expressions of the impulse and the
mental productions of an early stage of childish sexual life. And I
must remind you that my friend, Dr. C. G. Jung, read you a few hours
ago in this room an observation on a still younger girl who from the
same cause as my patient - the birth of a little child in the family
- betrayed certainly almost the same secret excitement, wish and
complex-creation. Accordingly I am not without hope that you may
feel friendly toward this idea of infantile sexuality that was so
strange at first.
I might also quote the remarkable example of the
Zurich psychiatrist, E. Bleuler, who said a few years ago openly
that he faced my sexual theories incredulous and bewildered, and
since that time by his own observations had substantiated them in
their whole scope. If it is true that most men, medical observers
and others, do not want to know anything about the sexual life of
the child, the fact is capable of explanation only too easily. They
have forgotten their own infantile sexual activity under the
pressure of education for civilization and do not care to be
reminded now of the repressed material. You will be convinced
otherwise if you begin the investigation by a self-analysis, by an
interpretation of your own childhood memories.
Lay aside your doubts and let us evaluate the
infantile sexuality of the earliest years. The sexual impulse of the
child manifests itself as a very complex one; it permits of an
analysis into many components, which spring from different sources.
It is entirely disconnected from the function of reproduction which
it is later to serve. It permits the child to gain different sorts
of pleasure sensations, which we include, by the analogues and
connections which they show, under the term sexual pleasures.
The great source of infantile sexual pleasure is the
auto-excitation of certain particularly sensitive parts of the body;
besides the genitals are included the rectum and the opening of the
urinary canal, and also the skin and other sensory surfaces. Since
in this first phase of child sexual life the satisfaction is found
on the child's own body and has nothing to do with any other object
we call this phase after a word coined by Havelock Ellis that of
"auto-eroticism." The parts of the body significant in giving sexual
pleasure we call "erogenous zones."
The thumb-sucking or passionate sucking of very young
children is a good example of such an auto-erotic satisfaction of an
erogenous zone. The first scientific observer of this phenomenon, a
specialist in children's diseases in Budapest by the name of
Lindner, interpreted these rightly as sexual satisfaction and
described exhaustively their transformation into other and higher
forms of sexual gratification.
Another sexual satisfaction of this time of life is
the excitation of the genitals by masturbation, which has such a
great significance for later life and, in the case of many
individuals, is never fully overcome. Besides this and other
auto-erotic manifestations we see very early in the child the
impulse-components of sexual pleasure, or, as we may say, of the libido, which
presupposes a second person as its object.
These impulses appear in opposed pairs, as active and
passive. The most important representatives of this group are the
pleasure in inflicting pain (sadism) with its passive exhibition -
pleasure. From the first of these later pairs splits off the
curiosity for knowledge, as from the latter impulse toward artistic
and theatrical representation.
Other sexual manifestations of the child can already
be regarded from the viewpoint of object-choice, in which the second
person plays the prominent part. The significance of this was
primarily based upon motives of the impulse of self-preservation.
The difference between the sexes plays, however, in the child no
very great role. One may attribute to every child, without wronging
him, a bit of the homosexual disposition.
The sexual life of the child, rich, but dissociated,
in which each single impulse goes about the business of arousing
pleasure independently of every other, is later correlated and
organized in two general directions, so that by
the close of puberty the definite sexual character of the individual
is practically, finally determined. The
single impulses subordinate themselves to the overlordship of the
genital zone, so that the whole sexual life is taken over into the
service of procreation, and their gratification is now significant
only so far as they help to prepare and promote the true sexual act.
On the other hand, object-choice prevails over
auto-eroticism, so that now in the sexual life all components of the
sexual impulse are satisfied in the loved person. But not all the
original impulse-components are given a share in the final shaping
of the sexual life. Even before the advent of puberty certain
impulses have undergone the most energetic repression under the
impulse of education, and mental forces like shame, disgust and
morality are developed, which, like sentinels, keep the repressed
wishes in subjection. When there comes, in puberty, the high tide of
sexual desire it finds dams in this creation of reactions and
resistances. These guide the outflow into the so-called normal
channels, and make it impossible to revivify the impulses which have
undergone repression.
The most important of these repressed impulses are coprophilism, that
is, the pleasure in children connected with the excrements; and,
further, the tendencies attaching themselves to the persons of the
primitive object-choice.
Gentlemen, a sentence of general pathology says that
every process of development brings with it the germ of pathological
dispositions in so far as it may be inhibited, delayed, or
incompletely carried out. This holds for the development of the
sexual function, with its many complications. It is not smoothly
completed in all individuals, and may leave behind either
abnormalities or disposition to later diseases by the way of later
falling back or regression. It
may happen that not all the partial impulses subordinate themselves
to the rule of the genital zone. Such an impulse which has remained
disconnected brings about what we call a perversion, which
may replace the normal sexual goal by one of its own.
It may happen, as has been said before, that the
auto-eroticism is not fully overcome, as many sorts of disturbances
testify. The originally equal value of both sexes as sexual objects
may be maintained and an inclination to homosexual activities in
adult life result from this, which, under suitable conditions, rises
to the level of exclusive homosexuality. This series of disturbances
corresponds to the direct inhibition of development of the sexual
function, it includes the perversions and the general infantilism of
the sex life that are not seldom met with.
The disposition to neuroses is
to be derived in another way from an injury to the development of
the sex life. The neuroses are related to the perversions as the
negative to the positive; in them we find the same
impulse-components as in perversions, as bearers of the complexes
and as creators of the symptoms; but here they work from out of the
unconscious. They have undergone a repression, but in spite of this
they maintain themselves in the unconscious.
Psychoanalysis teaches us that over-strong expression
of the impulse in very early life leads to a sort of fixation, which
then offers a weak point in the articulation of the sexual function.
If the exercise of the normal sexual function meets with hindrances
in later life, this repression, dating from the time of development,
is broken through at just that point at which the infantile fixation
took place.
You will now perhaps make the objection: "But all
that is not sexuality." I have used the word in a very much wider
sense than you are accustomed to understand it. This I willingly
concede. But it is a question whether you do not rather use the word
in much too narrow a sense when you restrict it to the realm of
procreation. You sacrifice by that the understanding of perversions;
of the connection between perversion, neurosis, and normal sexual
life; and have no means of recognizing, in its true significance,
the easily observable beginning of the somatic and mental sexual
life of the child. But however you decide about the use of the word,
remember that the psychoanalyst understands sexuality in that full
sense to which he is led by the evaluation of infantile sexuality.
Now we turn again to the sexual development of the
child. We still have much to say here, since we have given more
attention to the somatic than to the mental expressions of the
sexual life. The primitive object-choice of the child, which is
derived from his need of help, demands our further interest. It
first attaches to all persons to whom he is accustomed, but soon
these give way in favor of his parents.
The relation of the child to his parents is, as both
direct observation of the child and later analytic investigation of
adults agree, not at all free from elements of sexual
accessory-excitation. The child takes both parents, and especially
one, as an object of his erotic wishes. Usually he follows in this
the stimulus given by his parents, whose tenderness has very clearly
the character of a sex manifestation, though inhibited so far as its
goal is concerned.
As a rule, the father prefers the daughter, the
mother the son; the child reacts to this situation, since, as son,
he wishes himself in the place of his father, as daughter, in the
place of the mother. The feelings awakened in these relations
between parents and children, and, as a resultant of them, those
among the children in relation to each other, are not only
positively of a tender, but negatively of an inimical sort. The
complex built up in this way is destined to quick repression, but it
still exerts a great and lasting effect from the unconscious. We
must express the opinion that this with its ramifications presents
the nuclear complex of every neurosis, and so we are prepared to
meet with it in a not less effectual way in the other fields of
mental life.
The myth of King Oedipus, who kills his father and
wins his mother as a wife is only the slightly altered presentation
of the infantile wish, rejected later by the opposing barriers of
incest. Shakespeare's tale of Hamlet rests on the same basis of an
incest complex, though better concealed. At the time when the child
is still ruled by the still unrepressed nuclear complex, there
begins a very significant part of his mental activity which serves
sexual interest.
He begins to investigate the question of where
children come from and guesses, more than adults imagine, of the
true relations by deduction from the signs which he sees. Usually
his interest in this investigation is awakened by the threat to his
welfare through the birth of another child in the family, in whom at
first he sees only a rival. Under the influence of the partial
impulses which are active in him he [the boy baby] arrives at a
number of "infantile sexual theories," as that the same male
genitals belong to both sexes, that children are conceived by eating
and born through the opening of the intestine, and that sexual
intercourse is to be regarded as an inimical act, a sort of
overpowering.
But just the unfinished nature of his sexual
constitution and the gaps in his knowledge brought about by the
hidden condition of the feminine sexual canal, cause the infant
investigator to discontinue his work as a failure. The facts of this
childish investigation itself as well as the infant sex theories
created by it are of determinative significance in the building of
the child's character, and in the content of his later neuroses.
It is unavoidable and quite normal that the child
should make his parents the objects of his first object-choice. But
his libido must
not remain fixed on these first chosen objects, but must take them
merely as a prototype and transfer from these to other persons in
the time of definite object-choice. The breaking loose of the child
from his parents is thus a problem impossible to escape if the
social virtue of the young individual is not to be impaired.
During the time that the repressive activity is
making its choice among the partial sexual impulses and later, when
the influence of the parents, which in the most essential way has
furnished the material for these repressions, is lessened, great
problems fall to the work of education, which at present certainly
does not always solve them in the most intelligent and economic way.
Gentlemen, do not think that with these explanations
of the sexual life and the sexual development of the child we have
too far departed from psychoanalysis and the cure of neurotic
disturbances. If you like, you may regard the psychoanalytic
treatment only as a continued education for the overcoming of
childhood-remnants.
V
Ladies and Gentlemen: With the discovery of infantile
sexuality and the tracing back of the neurotic symptoms to erotic
impulse-components we have arrived at several unexpected formulae
for expressing the nature and tendencies of neurotic diseases. We
see that the individual falls ill when in consequence of outer
hindrances or inner lack of adaptability the satisfaction of the
erotic needs in the sphere of reality is denied. We see that he then
flees to sickness, in order to find with its help a surrogate
satisfaction for that denied him.
We recognize that the symptoms of illness contain
fractions of the sexual activity of the individual, or his whole
sexual life, and we find in the turning away from reality the chief
tendency and also the chief injury of the sickness. We may guess
that the resistance of our patients against the cure is not a simple
one, but is composed of many motives. Not only does the ego of
the patient strive against the giving up of the repression by which
it has changed itself from its original constitution into its
present form, but also the sexual impulses may not renounce their
surrogate satisfaction so long as it is not certain that they can be
offered anything better in the sphere of reality.
The flight from the unsatisfying reality into what we
call, on account of its biologically injurious nature, disease, but
which is never without an individual gain in pleasure for the
patient, takes place over the path of regression, the
return to earlier phases of the sexual life, when satisfaction was
not lacking. This regression is seemingly a twofold one, a temporal, in
so far as the libido or
erotic need falls back to a temporally earlier stage of development,
and a formal, since
the original and primitive psychic means of expression are applied
to the expression of this need. Both sorts of regression focus in
childhood and have their common point in the production of an
infantile condition of sexual life.
The deeper you penetrate into the pathogenic of
neurotic diseases, the more the connection of neuroses with other
products of human mentality, even the most valuable, will be
revealed to you. You will be reminded that we men, with the high
claims of our civilization and under the pressure of our
repressions, find reality generally quite unsatisfactory and so keep
up a life of fancy in which we love to compensate for what is
lacking in the sphere of reality by the production of
wish-fulfillments.
In these phantasies is often contained very much of
the particular constitutional essence of personality and of its
tendencies, repressed in real life. The energetic and successful man
is he who succeeds, by dint of labor, in transforming his wish
fancies into reality. Where this is not successful in consequence of
the resistance of the outer world and the weakness of the
individual, there begins the turning away from reality. The
individual takes refuge in his satisfying world of fancy.
Under certain conditions it still remains possible
for him to find another connecting link between these fancies and
reality, instead of permanently becoming a stranger to it through
the regression into the infantile. If the individual who is
displeased with reality is in possession of that artistic talent
which is still a psychological riddle, he can transform his fancies
into artistic creations. So he escapes the fate of a neurosis and
wins back his connection with reality by this round-about way. Where
this opposition to the real world exists, but this valuable talent
fails or is insufficient, it is unavoidable that the libido, following
the origin of the fancies, succeeds by means of regression in
revivifying the infantile wishes and so producing a neurosis. The
neurosis takes, in our time, the place of the cloister, in which
were accustomed to take refuge all those whom life had undeceived or
who felt themselves too weak for life.
Let me give at this point the main result at which we
have arrived by the psychoanalytic investigation of neurotics,
namely, that neuroses have no peculiar psychic content of their own,
which is not also to be found in healthy states; or, as C. G. Jung
has expressed it, neurotics fall ill of the same complexes with
which we sound people struggle. It depends on quantitative
relationships, on the relations of the forces wrestling with each
other, whether the struggle leads to health, to a neurosis, or to
compensatory over-functioning.
Ladies and Gentlemen, I have still withheld from you
the most remarkable experience which corroborates our assumptions of
the sexual impulse-forces of neurotics. Every time that we treat a
neurotic psychoanalytically, there occurs in him the so-called
phenomenon of transfer,that
is, he applies to the person of the physician a great amount of
tender emotion, often mixed with enmity, which has no foundation in
any real relation, and must be derived in every respect from the old
childish-fancies of the patient which have become unconscious.
Every fragment of his emotive life, which can no
longer be called back into memory, is accordingly lived over by the
patient in his relations to the physician, and only by such a living
of them over in the "transfer" is he convinced of the existence and
the power of these unconscious sexual excitations. The symptoms,
which, to use a simile from chemistry, are the precipitates of
earlier love experiences (in the widest sense), can only be
dissolved in the higher temperature of the experience of transfer
and transformed into other psychic products. The physician plays in
this reaction, to use an excellent expression of S. Ferenczi, the
role of a catalytic
ferment, which
temporarily attracts to itself the affect which has become free in
the course of the process.
The study of transfer can also give you the key to
the understanding of hypnotic suggestion, which we at first used
with our patients as a technical means of investigation of the
unconscious. Hypnosis showed itself at that time to be a therapeutic
help, but a hindrance to the scientific knowledge of the real nature
of the case, since it cleared away the psychic resistances from a
certain field, only to pile them up in an unscalable wall at the
boundaries of this field.
You must not think that the phenomenon of transfer,
about which I can unfortunately say only too little here, is created
by the influence of the psychoanalytic treatment. The transfer
arises spontaneously in all human relations and in the relations of
the patient to the physician; it is everywhere the especial bearer
of therapeutic influences, and it works the stronger the less one
knows of its presence. Accordingly psychoanalysis does not create
it, it merely discloses it to consciousness, and avails itself of
it, in order to direct the psychic processes to the wished-for goal.
But I cannot leave the theme of transfer without
stressing the fact that this phenomenon is of decisive importance to
convince not only the patient, but also the physician. I know that
all my adherents were first convinced of the correctness of my views
through their experience with transfer, and I can very well conceive
that one may not win such a surety of judgment so long as he makes
no psychoanalysis, and so has not himself observed the effects of
transfer.
Ladies and Gentlemen, I am of the opinion that there
are, on the intellectual side, two hindrances to acknowledging the
value of the psychoanalytic viewpoint: first, the fact that we are
not accustomed to reckon with a strict determination of mental life,
which holds without exception, and, second, the lack of knowledge of
the peculiarities through which unconscious mental processes differ
from these conscious ones with which we are familiar.
One of the most widespread resistances against the
work of psychoanalysis with patients, as with persons in health,
reduces to the latter of the two moments. One is afraid of doing
harm by psychoanalysis; one is anxious about calling up into
consciousness the repressed sexual impulses of the patient, as
though there were danger that they could overpower the higher
ethical strivings and rob him of his cultural acquisitions. One can
see that the patient has sore places in his soul life, but one is
afraid to touch them, lest his suffering be increased.
We may use this analogy. It is, of course, better not
to touch diseased places when one can only cause pain. But we know
that the surgeon does not refrain from the investigation and
reinvestigation of the seat of illness, if his invasion has as its
aim the restoration of lasting health. Nobody thinks of blaming him
for the unavoidable difficulties of the investigation or the
phenomena of reaction from the operation, if these only accomplish
their purpose, and gain for the patient a final cure by temporarily
making his condition worse.
The case is similar in psychoanalysis; it can lay
claim to the same things as surgery; the increase of pain which
takes place in the patient during the treatment is very much less
than that which the surgeon imposes upon him, and especially
negligible in comparison with the pains of serious illness. But the
consequence which is feared, that of a disturbance of the cultural
character by the impulse which has been freed from repression, is
wholly impossible.
In relation to this anxiety we must consider what our
experiences have taught us with certainty, that the somatic and
mental power of a wish, if once its repression has not succeeded, is
incomparably stronger when it is unconscious than when it is
conscious, so that by being made conscious it can only be weakened.
The unconscious wish cannot be influenced, is free from all
strivings in the contrary direction, while the conscious is
inhibited by those wishes which are also conscious and which strive
against it. The work of psychoanalysis accordingly presents a better
substitute, in the service of the highest and most valuable cultural
strivings, for the repression which has failed.
Now what is the fate of the wishes which have become
free by psychoanalysis, by what means shall they be made harmless
for the life of the individual? There are several ways. The general
consequence is that the wish is consumed during the work by the
correct mental activity of those better tendencies which are opposed
to it. The repression is supplanted by a condemnation carried
through with the best means at one's disposal.
This is possible, since for the most part we have to
abolish only the effects of earlier development stages of the ego.
The individual for his part only repressed the useless impulse,
because at that time he was himself still incompletely organized and
weak; in his present maturity and strength he can, perhaps, conquer
without injury to himself that which is inimical to him.
A second issue of the work of psychoanalysis may be
that the revealed unconscious impulses can now arrive at those
useful applications which, in the case of undisturbed developments,
they would have found earlier. The extirpation of the infantile
wishes is not at all the ideal aim of development. The neurotic has
lost, by his repressions, many sources of mental energy whose
contingents would have been very valuable for his character building
and his life activities.
We know a far more purposive process of development,
to so- called sublimation, by
which the energy of infantile wish-excitations is not secluded, but
remains capable of application, while for the particular
excitations, instead of becoming useless, a higher, eventually to
longer sexual, goal is set up. The components of the sexual instinct
are especially distinguished by such a capacity for the sublimation
and exchange of their sexual goal for one more remote and socially
more valuable. To the contributions of the energy won in such a way
for the functions of our mental life, we probably owe the highest
cultural consequences. A repression taking place at an early period
excludes the sublimation of the repressed impulse; after the removal
of the repression the way to sublimation is again free.
We must not neglect, also, to glance at the third of
the possible issues. A certain part of the suppressed libidinous
excitation has a right to direct satisfaction and ought to find it
in life. The claims of our civilization make life too hard for the
greater part of humanity, and so further the aversion to reality and
the origin of neuroses, without producing an excess of cultural gain
by this excess of sexual repression. We ought not to go so far as to
fully neglect the original animal part of our nature, we ought not
to forget that the happiness of individuals cannot be dispensed with
as one of the aims of our culture.
The plasticity of the sexual-components, manifest in
their capacity for sublimation, may cause a great temptation to
accomplish greater culture-effects by a more and more far-reaching
sublimation. But just as little as with our machines, we expect to
change more than a certain fraction of the applied heat into useful
mechanical work, just as little ought we to strive to separate the
sexual impulse in its whole extent of energy from its peculiar goal.
This cannot succeed, and if the narrowing of sexuality is pushed too
far it will have all the evil effects of a robbery.
I do not know whether you will regard the exhortation
with which I close as a presumptuous one. I only venture the
indirect presentation of my conviction, if I relate an old tale,
whose application you may make yourselves. German literature knows a
town called Schilda, to whose inhabitants were attributed all sorts
of clever pranks. The wiseacres, so the story goes, had a horse,
with whose powers of work they were well satisfied, and against whom
they had only one grudge, that he consumed so much expensive oats.
They concluded that by good management they would break him of this
bad habit, by cutting down his rations by several stalks each day,
until he had learned to do without them altogether. Things went
finely for a while, the horse was weaned to one stalk a day, and on
the next day he would at last work without fodder. On the morning of
this day the malicious horse was found dead; the citizens of Schilda
could not understand why he had died. We should be inclined to
believe that the horse had starved, and that without a certain
ration of oats no work would be expected from an animal.
I thank you for this opportunity and for the
attention that you have given me.
(Published in German by Franz Deuticke, Leipzig
and Vienna. 2nd. Edn. 1912. English translation: American Journal of
Psychology, Vol. 21, April, 1910. - Some errors and omissions
corrected here.)
Sigmund Freud graduated as Doctor of Medicine from
the Medical School of the University of Vienna in 1881. The LL.D.
was an honorary degree awarded by Clark University in 1909.